Skip to main content

A molecular genetic study of autism and related phenotypes in extended pedigrees

Abstract

Background

Efforts to uncover the risk genotypes associated with the familial nature of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have had limited success. The study of extended pedigrees, incorporating additional ASD-related phenotypes into linkage analysis, offers an alternative approach to the search for inherited ASD susceptibility variants that complements traditional methods used to study the genetics of ASD.

Methods

We examined evidence for linkage in 19 extended pedigrees ascertained through ASD cases spread across at least two (and in most cases three) nuclear families. Both compound phenotypes (i.e., ASD and, in non-ASD individuals, the broad autism phenotype) and more narrowly defined components of these phenotypes, e.g., social and repetitive behavior, pragmatic language, and anxiety, were examined. The overarching goal was to maximize the aggregate information available on the maximum number of individuals and to disaggregate syndromic phenotypes in order to examine the genetic underpinnings of more narrowly defined aspects of ASD behavior.

Results

Results reveal substantial between-family locus heterogeneity and support the importance of previously reported ASD loci in inherited, familial, forms of ASD. Additional loci, not seen in the ASD analyses, show evidence for linkage to the broad autism phenotype (BAP). BAP peaks are well supported by multiple subphenotypes (including anxiety, pragmatic language, and social behavior) showing linkage to regions overlapping with the compound BAP phenotype. Whereas 'repetitive behavior’, showing the strongest evidence for linkage (Posterior Probability of Linkage = 62% at 6p25.2-24.3, and 69% at 19p13.3), appears to be linked to novel regions not detected with other compound or narrow phenotypes examined in this study.

Conclusions

These results provide support for the presence of key features underlying the complexity of the genetic architecture of ASD: substantial between-family locus heterogeneity, that the BAP appears to correspond to sets of subclinical features segregating with ASD within pedigrees, and that different features of the ASD phenotype segregate independently of one another. These findings support the additional study of larger, even more individually informative pedigrees, together with measurement of multiple, behavioral- and biomarker-based phenotypes, in both affected and non-affected individuals, to elucidate the complex genetics of familial ASD.

Background

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD), a neurodevelopmental condition associated with lifelong disability, has now become an urgent public health challenge. Research into the genetics of ASD is motivated by the very real possibility that genetic testing can play a role in improving the diagnostic process and in providing targets for the development of new drugs [1]. However, much remains to be learned about the genetics of ASD before these goals are realized.

Early family studies based on retrospective data found a 2–9% recurrence of ASD among siblings of affected probands [2]. Recent work using prospective data and more up to date criteria [3] observed closer to 1 in 4 siblings affected – roughly 25 times higher than in the general population. Not only is the disorder familial, but variation in phenotypes such as IQ and language appear to be correlated among affected siblings as well [46]. Twin studies demonstrate that concordance for a phenotype that includes both autism and milder cognitive and social-communication deficits and rigidity (termed the broader autism phenotype or BAP), was >80% among monozygotic twins, compared with ~10% in dizygotic twin pairs [7], although the dizygotic rate is substantially higher in the most recent twin study [8]. This BAP also appears more common in relatives (particularly parents) of ASD probands than controls (20% vs. <10%) and more commonly in male than female relatives of ASD probands [911]. A recent prospective study of high-risk infant siblings indicates that this same extended phenotype (BAP traits, cognitive delays, and anxious behaviors) can be seen in upwards of 20% of infant siblings (who do not go on to develop ASD) compared to 3% in low-risk controls [12]. These combined findings indicate that ASD is a familial disorder (with roughly 40% of siblings showing either ASD or an extended BAP) with a strong genetic component.

However, studies attempting to uncover the risk genotype associated with the familial nature of the disorder have been largely unsuccessful. A review of linkage studies using mostly affected sibling pairs has indicated many 'significant’ linkage peaks with some replication among linkage signals [13]. Fine mapping under the peaks has not been successful in uncovering ASD genes, likely due to, among other reasons, a degree of allelic heterogeneity that, in retrospect, was greater than anticipated and perhaps by an over-reliance on the compound phenotype of ASD as opposed to a more modular approach. Four large independent genome-wide association studies have been reported, but so far the data indicate that few, if any, common variants have a substantial impact on risk [1417]. A recent cross-disorder genome-wide association study reports more promising results, but specificity of those common variants to ASD is unclear [18].

In startling contrast, there is substantial evidence that a multitude of rare de novo copy number variants (CNVs) contribute, at least in part, to the etiology of sporadic (i.e., non-familial) ASD [19, 20]. To quantify the role of CNVs, a series of large studies (for a review see [21]), have used microarrays to interrogate ASD cohorts. Although estimates vary among studies, between 2% to 10% of probands appear to have de novo structural variants [21, 22]. Every risk locus or gene so far occurs with less than 1% frequency in ASD cases.

The field has been more likely to assign a causal role to de novo CNVs than to inherited ones in spite of the fact that ASD is a familial disorder and that rare inherited CNVs have also been consistently reported in ASD. Several pedigrees showing inherited CNVs or point mutations in a number of key CNS genes or regions (SHANK1, CDH8, NRXN3, PTCHD1, 16p11.2) have also been published [2327]. In many of these, the transmitting parent had the BAP or a related phenotype; for example, in a pedigree transmitting a SHANK1 deletion, we determined that the transmitting parents (both mothers) had suffered from anxiety disorders [26] (such disorders were not present in non-transmitting relatives who we were able to assess). In other pedigrees, medical/congenital or cognitive abnormalities in relatives also appear to be segregating with a rare variant. However, these represent single case reports and it is therefore hard to argue that these phenotypes are variable expressions of the ASD genotype at the population level as opposed to the individual case level.

The study of extended pedigrees (defined here as including relatives outside of the nuclear family identified by an ASD proband) offers an opportunity to study the inherited nature of ASD more thoroughly. Such pedigrees may have enough information to be able to follow the segregation of a genetic variant through a single family to see if it is associated with phenotype status. Several groups have employed the extended pedigree strategy, ranging from a search for shared CNVs across cousin pairs to studying a single, extensive Finnish pedigree from 20 nuclear families [2832]. One study incorporated measurement of the social responsiveness scale subphenotype into the analysis of extended pedigrees and concluded that this was a robust and useful approach for exploring genetic linkage in studies of ASD [30]. If an important goal is to understand the familial nature of ASD, a greater focus on inherited variants is warranted employing a broad range of phenotypes including those segregating in parents and siblings without ASD. However, there are no systematic linkage studies on whether non-ASD individuals with a potentially pathogenic variant have any phenotype of interest such as the BAP and related phenotypes.

Incorporating additional phenotypes into linkage analysis may be a robust way to uncover inherited ASD susceptibility variants. Consider a genetic mutation that increases the risk of ASD and which can also produce a sub-clinical phenotype in mutation carriers. Misclassifying carriers as 'unaffected’ artificially reduces penetrance estimates for linkage analysis, while misclassifying non-carriers as 'affected’ introduces apparent phenocopies; either of these errors will tend to depress linkage signals. Thus, in a set of pedigrees segregating this mutation, the more accurately the sub-clinical phenotype is measured, the stronger the linkage evidence should be. In other words, a clinical definition that correctly captures the segregation pattern of the mutation will, all other things being equal, result in stronger linkage evidence. Similarly, a quantitative trait (QT) that correctly segregates with the mutation will produce stronger linkage evidence, in general, than one that does not. Here, we utilize clinical data on several ASD-related phenotypes in conjunction with linkage analysis of extended pedigrees to explore genotype-phenotype relationships in ASD.

Methods

Participants

We recruited extended pedigrees with at least three ASD cases spread across at least two nuclear families (in all but one case, the three cases were spread across three nuclear families). All families were either known to the authors through previous studies or identified through advertising. In all, 19 families were available for this study, 6 recruited in Canada (CA) and 13 in the US. The CA pedigrees had an average of 24 genotyped individuals and 25 phenotyped individuals, while the US pedigrees had an average of 16 genotyped individuals and 18 phenotyped individuals. To minimize etiologic heterogeneity, families were excluded from the study if there was evidence of the following co-occurring medical conditions thought to be etiologically-related to autism in one of the index probands with autism: tuberous sclerosis, neurofibromatosis, phenylketonuria, Fragile X screening, or significant CNS injury. We did not exclude individuals with a chromosome abnormality in order to determine whether that abnormality might also be inherited and play a role in susceptibility. All individuals were of northern European heritage. All data collection took place under Institutional Review Board approval and the research was conducted in accordance with the World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki [33]. Written informed consent was obtained from all subjects after the study had been fully explained.

Clinical methods

Our overarching clinical strategy included clinical assessments performed to i) index eligible extended pedigrees, by identifying at least three related individuals with a Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (4th ed) (DSM-IV) pervasive developmental disorder; and to ii) characterize all pedigree members on phenotypes of interest. For the latter goal of characterizing pedigree members, the strategy employed was to assess for both compound phenotypes (i.e., ASD or, in non-ASD individuals, the BAP) and more narrowly defined components of these phenotypes including social and repetitive behavior, pragmatic language, and anxiety. The overarching goal in taking this multi-tiered approach was to maximize the aggregate information available on the maximum number of affected individuals (i.e., global ratings of ASD or the BAP) as well as to disaggregate these global phenotypes to look at the genetic underpinnings of individual behavioral aspects of the broad construct of autistic behavior.

Overlapping sets of instruments were used to diagnose ASD and the BAP in the CA and US pedigrees, respectively. After initial screening based on the Telephone Screening Interview, the Autism Family History Interview [34], and review of medical records, diagnosis was based on expert clinical judgment incorporating information from the Autism Diagnostic Interview Revised [35] and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule – Revised [36], administered by trained and reliable clinicians. All participants classified as ASD met DSM IV criteria [37] for either Autistic disorder, Asperger syndrome, or pervasive developmental disorder not otherwise specified according to the criteria in Risi et al. [38]. The US extended pedigrees included 3 to 6 individuals with ASD (average = 4); while the CA extended pedigrees included 3 to 8 (average = 5).

Non-ASD family members were assessed for BAP, which broadly covers the domains of aloof personality, rigid personality, and pragmatic language deficits. The Modified Personality Assessment Schedule Revised (MPAS-R) [39, 40] and Modified Pragmatic Rating Scale (MPRS) [39, 40], which are the preferred methods for BAP assessment in individuals aged 16 or over, were administered only to the US families. The MPAS-R [41] is a semi-structured interview for rating personality characteristics adapted from the Personality Assessment Schedule [42], further revised to assess six personality characteristics: aloof, anxious, hypersensitive, overly-conscientious, rigid, and untactful [40]. In the MPAS-R, information is collected via separate self and informant interviews and the characteristics are rated as either present, absent or unknown [39] by two independent clinicians, with a third rater serving as a tiebreaker when needed. For this study, only two MPAS-R characteristics (aloof and rigid) were utilized in determining the BAP, although all characteristics were queried and rated to maintain the integrity of the instrument. Aloof and rigid have consistently been the most valid and reliable at distinguishing relatives of individuals with autism from relatives of typically developing individuals in our previous studies (e.g., Piven et al. [40]). The MPRS is an abbreviated version of the pragmatic rating scale (PRS) [43], developed to identify seven pragmatic language skills and four prosodic and grammatical speech errors with more efficiency and less redundancy than the PRS. These eleven items were extracted from the PRS using logistic regression to determine those that most reliably predict pragmatic speech deficits; they best differentiated parents of children with autism and parents of typically developing children in the Iowa Family Study [40], are highly correlated to the PRS, and were validated during the Collaborative Linkage Study of Autism [44]. Trained interviewers rate items 0, 1, or 2 based on a guided conversation incorporated in the MPAS-R that ensures that all rated behaviors have the opportunity to be observed. The PRS has good inter-rater reliability (κ = 0.77) [40, 43].

When MPAS-R and MPRS consensus ratings were not available, as for all CA pedigrees and some US individuals, the BAP Questionnaire (BAP-Q) [45] was used for diagnosis of the BAP and components (aloof and rigid personality, and pragmatic language deficits). The BAP-Q was also used as a screening tool and mailed to participants to determine whether or not to follow-up with more comprehensive BAP assessment or to gather 'diagnostic’ information on participants who were otherwise unavailable for in-person visits. The BAP-Q is a self- and informant-report questionnaire consisting of 36 items spread across three 12-item subscales derived from direct assessment interviews (social aloofness and rigid personality from the MPAS-R and pragmatic language abnormalities from the MPRS). Items are rated along a six-point Likert scale (ranging from 'very rarely’ to 'very often’), which forces ratings to fall above or below a value of neutral on each question. Original internal consistency analysis of the subscales [45] supported this three-component model, which is consistent with the traditional conceptualizations of domains characterizing autism: social, communication and restricted and repetitive behaviors. The measure was completed by the participant about him/herself (the self-version) and by someone close to the participant about him/her (the informant version) to obtain an average score (between the self and informant scores); whenever available, the average scores were utilized. A BAP diagnosis was assigned if an individual met gender-specific criteria in any domain [46] (see Table 1 for cut-offs). These diagnostic cut-offs are higher than those originally published in 2007 and have higher specificity than the 2007 cut-offs now suggested for use in screening. The US pedigrees had a range of 2 to 10 BAP positive cases (average = 5); the CA pedigrees had a range of 1 to 6 BAP positive cases (average = 4). Note that when analyzing ASD alone, BAP positive individuals were coded as unaffected.

Table 1 Cut-off scores for BAP-Q self and informant ratings, and the average of the two, for males and females

The US (but not CA) families were additionally assessed on seven secondary phenotypes. The secondary US phenotypes address various aspects of autism and include anxiety (ANX), repetitive/ritualistic behavior (RRB), social functioning (SOC), and language abnormalities, i.e., social communication (PRS), core language ability (CLF), non-word repetition (NWR), and rapid naming (RAP). The CL, NWR, and RAP phenotypes were considered exploratory as there is less support for these constructs showing familiality in the literature, and were added to enrich the overall assessment for communication phenotype information, one of three major criteria for autism in the DSM IV.

ANX, RRB, and SOC are quantitative variables. ANX is based on the anxiety (N1) facet t-score of the NEO Personality Inventory Revised [47], given to non-ASD family members aged 17 and over (reported to aggregate in parents of autistic probands [40]). Since ANX was not measured in ASD cases, a quantitative trait threshold model was used, designating ASD cases as 'over threshold’ and non-cases as under (when no ANX score was available). Observed ANX has a bell-shaped distribution with an overall mean score of 51 (SD = 9), and the within-pedigree means ranged from 47 to 63. RRB is the overall score from the Repetitive Behavior Scale Revised [48], and has an overall mean of 13 (SD = 16) and is higher in ASD cases (mean = 26, SD = 16) than non-cases (mean = 4, SD = 7). Mean pedigree RRB varies greatly from 6 to 31. SOC is the total t-score from the Social Responsiveness Scale [49]. The SOC distribution is skewed right with an overall mean of 55 (SD = 18) and is also greater in ASD cases (mean = 84, SD = 14) than non-cases (mean = 48, SD = 11). Mean pedigree SOC is fairly stable with a range of 48 to 67.

The remaining variables are dichotomous. PRS is based on ASD status and the social score of the MPRS, used above to diagnose BAP; 61% of those assessed with MPRS were positive on PRS and 24% of non-ASD cases were positive on PRS. All ASD positive individuals were considered as affected for PRS. The pedigrees have between 6 and 10 PRS cases. CLF affection indicates a standard core language score from the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals 4th Edition below 84 (1 SD below the mean) [50]. Half of ASD cases were positive on CLF while only 2% of non-cases were positive. Families had at most three CLF cases. NWR and RAP are phenotypes derived from performance on the Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing [51]: NWR cases have non-word repetition subtest standard scores ≤7 (1 SD below the mean), and RAP cases have rapid naming composite scores <84 (1 SD below the mean) [52]. For NWR, 41% of ASD cases were positive compared to 23% of non-cases. Similarly for RAP, 46% of ASD cases were positive versus 11% of non-cases. The extended pedigrees have at most 9 or 7 cases of NWR or RAP, respectively, but there are pedigrees with none for both phenotypes.

Genotyping and data cleaning

Three hundred and twenty-two individuals (range 7–32, mean 17/pedigree) were genotyped using a dense microsatellite (MS) marker set (avg. intermarker distance 4 cM) obtained from DeCode; 280 individuals (range 6–29, mean 15/pedigree) were also genotyped using the Illumina OMNI 2.5 M chip. In preparation for linkage analysis, both MS and SNP data were used to verify family structure; founders were assessed for relatedness (no relatedness found), and extended relationships were confirmed. All genotypes were cleaned for marker missingness (dropping markers above thresholds of 5% for SNPs, 25% for MSs), sample missingness (>5% SNP; >25% MS), and excess Mendel errors both by marker and individual; SNPs with a Hardy-Weinberg p value <1x10-10 and MSs below 1x10-4 were dropped. After thinning of the SNP map to remove marker-marker linkage disequilibrium (R2 >0.20), we based linkage analyses on a combined map comprising 10,364 SNPs + 1,078 MSs. The genetic map was based on the Build 36 hg18 (Build 37 hg19 used for X only) Rutgers Combined Linkage-Physical Map (http://compgen.rutgers.edu/RutgersMap) [53] (custom release November 2011). The full set of (pre-thinned) SNPs was also used to call CNVs. No CNVs were observed to segregate within pedigrees and they are therefore not further discussed in this paper.

Statistical methods

Linkage analysis was conducted using the software package Kelvin, which implements the PPL class of models for measuring the strength of genetic evidence [54, 55]. (“PPL” originally stood for “posterior probablity of linkage.”) In order to take advantage of the very dense marker coverage and given the size of the pedigrees, MCMC was used to calculate marker likelihoods as described in [56], while Kelvin’ s non-stochastic algorithm was used to calculate trait likelihoods conditional on marker data [57]. We note that these calculations are highly computationally intensive, requiring approximately 36,000 CPU hours (1-month real time) with calculations distributed over a 90-node Linux cluster with 16–128 Gb memory/node.

Three different trait models were employed, depending on the type of trait being analyzed: dichotomous trait (DT, used for ASD, BAP, CLF, NWR, PRS and RAP), quantitative trait (QT, used for RRB, SOC), and QT threshold (QTT, used for ANX). The DT model is parameterized in terms of α (the admixture parameter of Smith [58], representing the proportion of 'linked’ pedigrees), p (the disease allele frequency), and the penetrance vector f i , representing the probability that an individual with genotype i develops disease, for i – 1..3. The QT model replaces the penetrances with a vector of three genotypic means and three genotypic variances. Note that normality is assumed at the genotypic level, but not at the population level, and there is no inflation of scores under violations of normality [59]. The QTT model extends the QT model by allowing for 'affected’ individuals, who are missing QT values but assumed to have exceeded some threshold on the QT scale, with the threshold itself being an additional parameter of the model. All trait parameters are integrated out of the final statistic, using essentially uniform prior distributions (ordering constraints are imposed on the penetrances or means), and unbounded parameters are integrated over a finite range [54, 59], implicitly allowing for dominant, recessive, and additive models. This provides a robust approximation for mapping complex traits in terms of the marginal model at each locus, and because the parameters are integrated out, no specific assumptions regarding their values are required.

The PPL has two basic approaches to the accumulation of evidence, which we employ here to consider evidence across pedigrees. Under 'pooled’ (PPLPOOL), the trait parameters are integrated across all pedigrees as a set at each locus. This is appropriate under the expectation that at each locus, the trait model is essentially the same across pedigrees. Under 'sequential’ (PPLSEQ), trait parameters are integrated over separately for each pedigree at each locus, and the marginal evidence for or against linkage itself is accumulated across pedigrees using Bayesian sequential updating. Sequential updating is appropriate under the expectation that each pedigree may implicate different loci and/or the same loci but under different trait models (as could arise, e.g., in the presence of important background genetic and/or environmental modification). When there is relative genetic homogeneity, pooling will yield larger signals at linked loci; when there is extensive heterogeneity, however, sequential will yield larger signals at linked loci and also smaller signals at unlinked ones [60].

Since the PPL framework differs from standard frequentist statistical approaches, a few comments on its interpretation will be helpful. First, the PPL is on the probability scale, and its interpretation is therefore straightforward, e.g., PPL = 40%, means that there is an estimated 40% probability of a trait gene at the given location based on the data. The only caveat to this interpretation is that this estimated probability is influenced by the low prior probability of linkage (π) to any given locus. Based on empirical data [61] we set π = 2% (this assumes just one disease gene in the genome and is thus conservative, possibly highly conservative, under locus heterogeneity). Thus PPL >2% indicates (some degree) of evidence in favor of a trait gene at that locus, while PPL <2% represents evidence against the location. As with any Bayesian method, the influence of this small prior probability on the final PPL can be appreciable until the data set becomes large. Hence, even a small PPL – say, 20% – indicates that the data are supporting linkage enough to make the posterior probability 10 times larger than the prior. Hence, in finite samples, we do not interpret 1-PPL as the probability of no gene; rather, we interpret the PPL relative to the prior probability π = 2%.

Additional distinctive features of the statistical framework are related to the fact that the PPL is a measure of statistical evidence, not a decision-making procedure. There are, therefore, no 'significance levels’ associated with it (i.e., no specific cut-offs beyond which we declare significance) and it is not interpreted in terms of associated error probabilities [62, 63]. By the same token, no multiple testing corrections are applied to the PPL, just as one would not 'correct’ a measure of the temperature made in one location for temperature readings taken at different locations [64].

Results

The results are presented in four sections: i) we consider the comparison between pooled and sequentially updated results for both ASD and BAP, in order to gauge the extent of between-family heterogeneity, and then present primary ii) ASD and iii) BAP results, followed by iv) results for the secondary phenotypes.

i) Pooling vs. sequential updating

Figure 1a shows pooled and sequential results across the genome for ASD, while Figure 1b shows results for BAP. Within each sub-figure, results are correlated, as expected. However, for ASD, there was just 1 PPLPOOL ≥0.20, but 4 under sequential (max PPLSEQ = 0.41, at the same position as PPLPOOL = 0.20); while for BAP the corresponding numbers were 1 (PPLPOOL = 0.41) and 5 (with max PPL = 0.46 at that same position). Across the board, where PPL >10%, PPLSEQ > PPLPOOL (with one exception on chromosome 12 for BAP, where PPLPOOL = 0.20 and PPLSEQ = 0.17). At the same time, the proportion of the genome showing evidence against linkage (PPL <2%) was 74% and 79% for ASD pooled and sequential, respectively, and 66% and 70% for BAP pooled and sequential, respectively. Thus, for both phenotypes, a greater proportion of the genome showed evidence against linkage under sequential analysis, and at these locations, the evidence against linkage tended to be stronger. This pattern of results is consistent with appreciable genetic heterogeneity across pedigrees. In what follows, we therefore rely on sequential as the primary data analytic approach to the accumulation of evidence going forward, dropping the 'SEQ’ subscript.

Figure 1
figure 1

Genome scans for (a) ASD and (b) BAP comparing 'pooled’ with 'sequentially updated’ results. Note that for visual clarity, the y-axis goes from 0.0–0.5, rather than 0.0–1.0.

ii) ASD results (sequential)

Four loci show PPL >20% (1p36.32, PPL = 30%; 2q37.2, PPL = 26%; 15q12, PPL = 22%; 22q13.31, PPL = 41%), with additional loci on chromosomes 2, 8, 9, and possibly 12 standing out above the background as well (Figure 1a). Not surprisingly, no single pedigree generates large PPLs on its own. We note however, that on 8q12.1 a modest sequential peak appears to be driven by a single pedigree (PPL = 19% in Ped 4); because the sequential peak is somewhat lower than this, we can conclude that the remaining pedigrees in aggregate show some evidence against linkage to this locus. On 15q11.2, a single pedigree (Ped 16) shows PPL = 15% on its own; however, the sequential peak is >15%, suggesting that while this one pedigree accounts for the preponderance of the evidence, one or more additional pedigrees must also be supporting linkage at this locus. By contrast, PPLs of comparable size on 16q23.1-q23.2 (PPL = 19% in Ped 16) and Xp22.11-p21.3 (PPL = 18% in Ped 5) are almost completely erased in the sequential results. This is consistent with the possibility of major loci within each individual pedigree not found in any of the remaining pedigrees.

iii) BAP results (sequential)

Five additional loci, not seen in the ASD analyses, show BAP PPL >20% (Figure 1b): 2q37.3 (PPL = 47%), 11q23.3 (PPL = 20%), 14q11.2 (PPL = 29%), 14q31.3 (PPL = 32%) and 15q13.3 (PPL = 20%). Considering individual pedigrees (Figure 2b), just one salient peak emerges, and this is on 22q13.32 (PPL = 21% in Ped 4). Interestingly, while that locus was salient in the omnibus (all pedigrees) ASD scan, under ASD this particular pedigree gives evidence against linkage (PPL <2%) at this locus.

Figure 2
figure 2

Genome scans for (a) ASD and (b) BAP, by individual pedigree. Note that the y-axis goes from 0.0–0.25.

Table 2 shows salient results for both ASD and BAP; Figure 3 shows ASD and BAP plots for selected chromosomes. Not surprisingly, at most loci, results are correlated; at only 2 loci (11q23.2, 22q13.31) does one phenotype show evidence for and the other against linkage. Notably, of the remaining 5 loci, in only one case (1p36.32) is the ASD score substantially higher than the BAP score, while at 5 of 7 loci the BAP score is substantially higher than the ASD score.

Figure 3
figure 3

ASD and BAP results for selected chromosomes. Note that the y-axis goes from 0.0–0.5.

Table 2 Primary ASD and BAP findings

iv) Results for additional phenotypes

Figure 4 shows genome-wide results for each of the seven secondary phenotypes (US pedigrees only). There are multiple loci with signals greater than any seen in the BAP analyses (with the exception of 2q37.2-q37.3, which is higher for BAP than any of the sub-phenotypes). None of the exploratory phenotypes NWR, RAP, or CLF shows any signals >15% genome-wide; these phenotypes are not further considered here. It is important to keep in mind that due to the limited size of the pedigrees, results across phenotypes are expected to show some degree of correlation, whether the phenotypes are measuring the same underlying traits or not. Another way to express this is to note that considering multiple phenotypes in a single set of pedigrees is a form of permuting the phenotypes; combined with selection of loci based on maximum scores, this will tend to lead to 'inflation’ of linkage results [65]. Nevertheless, bearing this caveat in mind, some interesting patterns emerge.

Figure 4
figure 4

Genome scan for seven additional phenotypes, US pedigrees only.

First, there are 4 loci at which BAP has the highest PPL (Table 3). Of these, perhaps surprisingly, 3 loci are either not supported or only marginally supported by any of the other phenotypes. Only 1 BAP peak (2q37.2-q37.3) is well supported by multiple subphenotypes, with only RRB giving evidence against linkage across this locus.

Table 3 Results for BAP and secondary phenotypes for any locus with PPL >20% for at least one phenotype (US pedigrees only)

Second, ANX, PRS, and SOC show multiple peaks (4 PPLs >20% for ANX, 3 for PRS, 5 for SOC, at a total of 9 loci overall). Apart from 2q37.2-q37.3 (which is best supported by BAP), only 2 loci are strongly supported by 2 or more of these phenotypes (on 9q21.31-22.31, PRS and SOC with slight supporting evidence from ANX; on 13q11-q12.3 ANX and PRS with evidence against linkage for SOC). At the remaining loci, only one phenotype clearly supports linkage; particularly notable in this regard are 9p21.3 and 15q26.3, which are supported by ANX with both PRS and SOC giving evidence against linkage. Thus, in general, there does not seem to be a clear pattern of correlation across these phenotypes. On the contrary, while it is difficult to draw definitive conclusions based on these pedigrees alone, there is some suggestion that the three phenotypes might be picking up different underlying genetic loci. Focusing on the 5 loci with PPL >30% for either ANX, PRS, or SOC (in order to minimize noise due to permutation over the phenotypes): 8q24.13 is supported by SOC with ANX and PRS neutral; 9q21.31-q22.31 is supported by SOC as well as PRS and probably ANX; 13q11-q12.3 is supported by ANX as well as PRS but not SOC; 15q26.3 is supported by ANX but neither PRS or SOC; and 19p12-q12 is supported by SOC but not by ANX, with PRS neutral.

Finally, a striking conclusion emerges regarding RRB. RRB itself gives the two highest PPLs seen for any phenotypes (including in the primary analyses based on all of the pedigrees): PPL = 62% at 6p25.2-24.3, and PPL = 69% at 19p13.3. Each of these loci is at best very slightly supported by one additional phenotype, with the remaining phenotypes either neutral or giving evidence against linkage. Particularly notable is that even ASD and BAP do not support these loci (Figure 5). Furthermore, RRB itself gives evidence against linkage at virtually all of the other loci in Table 3 (with the exceptions of 6p25.3-p25.2 and 12q21.1-q21.33 where it is neutral (note that the maximum PPLs at these 2 loci are <30%), and this includes loci supported by BAP. Thus, clear evidence emerges that RRB, while highly informative for linkage in these pedigrees, which were ascertained for the presence of multiple cases of ASD, is in fact segregating independently of both BAP and the remaining subphenotypes, and even ASD itself.

Figure 5
figure 5

RRB shows two large signals which appear to be independent of ASD or BAP: (a) Genome scan for ASD, BAP and RRB (US pedigrees only); (b) Chromosome 6; (c) Chromosome 19.

Discussion

By utilizing a set of extended pedigrees and clinical assessments of a number of phenotypes in both ASD and non-ASD relatives, we have taken an approach that differs from traditional methods used to study the genetics of ASD. This approach has yielded several interesting findings. First, the primary ASD results confirm roles for known ASD loci in extended pedigrees. The four salient loci include the Prader-Willi Angelman region on 15q and the 22q13 deletion syndrome region, both well-established syndromes associated with autistic features in subsets of cases; in addition, 2q37 contains CENTG2, which has obtained modest support as an ASD gene in independent studies [66], and 1p36 is associated with 1p36 deletion syndrome. While considering additional loci highlighted in the analysis of individual pedigrees risks the appearance of 'cherry picking’, here too the findings highlight overlap with previously identified loci, particularly at 8q12.1, which is the ICHD7/CHARGE syndrome region, and Xp22, which covers PTCHD1 [24, 67]. While these loci have been highlighted before, they have been discovered primarily through individual case studies or de novo CNVs. Our results support the importance of these loci in inherited, highly familial, forms of ASD as well.

Secondly, in aggregate, our results strongly support the presence of substantial between-pedigree locus heterogeneity for both ASD and BAP analyses. Assuming common loci across pedigrees (PPLPOOL) produced lower PPLs at all loci with PPL >20% compared to allowing for different genetic loci and models across pedigrees (PPLSEQ) (PPLSEQ also returned evidence against linkage across a larger proportion of the genome). Moreover, as previously mentioned, support was found for some previously implicated ASD loci in one individual pedigree but not others. This pattern is consistent with results from the CNV analyses published over the last decade that have shown very little sharing of loci across different families. This highlights the need for locus- and gene-discovery methods that are robust to locus heterogeneity, and should inform our interpretation of negative as well as positive findings going forward.

A third notable finding is that, while some peaks are better supported by ASD and others by BAP, in only two cases does a salient peak under one diagnosis show evidence against linkage under the other. In general, adding the BAP to the analysis does appear to confirm the hypothesis that the BAP is of relevance to ASD genetics. Of particular interest, perhaps, are the BAP findings at 2q37.3 and 11q23.2, the first of which contains UBE2F and the second UBE4A. Note that 11q was also reported by Liu et al. [68] in an independent data set.

The fourth interesting finding involves the phenotypes ANX, PRS, and/or SOC, which provided multiple loci of interest. Given correlations across phenotypes within pedigrees, we are sensitive to the speculative nature of any conclusions regarding genetic relationships between phenotypes. However, the patterns of results are consistent with a model in which these sub-phenotypes are segregating independently of one another at some loci, and perhaps independently of BAP in some 'carrier’ relatives, but still be involved in the etiology of ASD if other events or 'hits’ at other loci added to the genetic liability for ASD itself. Such interaction might be specific to particular pedigrees, to particular loci, or may generalize across pedigrees. This seems like a fruitful area of investigation for future family-based genetic studies. The fact that the exploratory phenotypes NWR, RAP, and CLF showed no notable evidence for linkage across the genome also serves as a kind of negative control, suggesting that not all phenotypes conceptually related to the defining features of autism are genetically relevant in these pedigrees. On the other hand, these traits may simply have been less informative in this particular data set; only 2, 5, and 7 families are multiplex for CLF, RAP and NWR, respectively. Additional data collection would be needed to resolve this.

The fifth finding of this study is that RRB appears to be entirely genetically independent of BAP and the remaining secondary phenotypes, yet highly informative in these families, yielding the two largest PPLs seen across all analyses. This is consistent with recent empirical measurement models that suggest that ASD is composed of two key phenotypes that co-occur [69]. Under this model, it remains to be explained why ASD itself does not support linkage to the RRB loci. It is certainly possible that the ASD phenotype alone is simply not informative enough at those loci; for instance, under a two-locus epistasis model, the meiotic information regarding RRB transmission might come primarily from non-ASD individuals, with parents of those who develop ASD tending to be homozygous for RRB genotypes and therefore uninformative. However, this model will remain entirely speculative until it is possible to identify the responsible genomic variants under the peaks.

The strengths of this study include the large number of extended pedigrees with high quality, highly informative marker data, careful attention to measuring multiple phenotypes for both ASD and non-ASD individuals within those pedigrees, and statistical methods, which allowed us to take full advantage of all features of the data. The salient limitation of the study is nevertheless sample size; despite the size of the data set, some phenotypes remained insufficiently informative, and particular relationships among phenotypes remain to be confirmed and further defined. Furthermore, the results themselves suggest considerable heterogeneity between pedigrees, yet each pedigree on its own is not sufficiently large to yield definitive linkage results. Hence, most of the signals reported here remain modest in size and undoubtedly not all of them represent true linkages (although the overlap with known ASD loci supports the efficacy of picking up true ASD genes at several of these moderately supported loci).

Finally, the phenotypes considered here are almost certainly still just proxies for more biologically defined underlying features. This complicates interpretation of results across phenotypes. For example, perhaps the most tantalizing result is the apparent independent segregation of RRB even from ASD within these ASD-multiplex families. This seems contradictory, unless ASD as diagnosed via the Autism Diagnostic Interview and Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADI and ADOS) is a measure for some aspects of autism but not all. It is worth noting here that the measurement of repetitive behaviors in the RBS-R is not equivalent to what is used for the ASD diagnosis itself; 'repetitive behaviors’ do not constitute a unitary construct, and more finely grained or biologically direct phenotypes that are likely to bring us closer to the complex mechanisms occurring in ASD, would help clarify the ASD-RRB relationship from a genetic point of view [70].

Conclusions

Overall, our primary conclusions speak to key features of the complexity of the genetic architecture of ASD: i) there appears to be substantial between-family locus heterogeneity; ii) in keeping with previous epidemiologic findings, the BAP does appear to correspond at least roughly to sets of subclinical features segregating with ASD within pedigrees, so that equating non-ASD relatives with non-carriers of ASD genes is not correct in general; and iii) different features of the ASD phenotype appear to segregate independently of one another within these pedigrees, in support of the multiple hit model articulated by Eichler and others [7173]. If these findings prove correct, they pose a set of challenges to future studies. We believe that these challenges will be met, at least in part, by the study of additional and larger, even more individually informative pedigrees, together with measurement of multiple, and perhaps more biologically direct, phenotypes in both affected and non-affected individuals. Pedigrees of the type needed for these studies are uncommon, but they do exist. Exploiting the many opportunities they provide to further our understanding of the complex genetics of ASD seems both possible and extremely promising.

Abbreviations

ANX:

Anxiety

ASD:

Autism spectrum disorder

BAP:

Broad autism phenotype

BAP-Q:

BAP Questionnaire

CLF:

Core language ability

CNV:

Copy number variants

MPAS-R:

Modified personality assessment schedule revised

MPRS:

Modified pragmatic rating scale

MS:

Microsatellite

NWR:

Non-word repetition

PPL:

Posterior probability of linkage

PRS:

Pragmatic rating scale

QT:

Quantitative trait

RAP:

Rapid naming

RRB:

Repetitive/ritualistic behavior

SOC:

Social functioning.

References

  1. State MW, Sestan N: Neuroscience. The emerging biology of autism spectrum disorders. Science. 2012, 337 (6100): 1301-1303. 10.1126/science.1224989.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Ritvo ER, Jorde LB, Mason-Brothers A, Freeman BJ, Pingree C, Jones MB, McMahon WM, Petersen PB, Jenson WR, Mo A: The UCLA-University of Utah epidemiologic survey of autism: recurrence risk estimates and genetic counseling. Am J Psychiatry. 1989, 146 (8): 1032-1036.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  3. Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A, Messinger D, Yirmiya N, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Carver LJ, Constantino JN, Dobkins K, Hutman T, Iverson JM, Landa R, Rogers SJ, Sigman M, Stone WL: Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. Pediatrics. 2011, 128 (3): e488-e495.

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  4. Szatmari P, Jones MB, Holden J, Bryson S, Mahoney W, Tuff L, MacLean J, White B, Bartolucci G, Schutz C, Robinson P, Hoult L: High phenotypic correlations among siblings with autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Am J Med Genet. 1996, 67 (4): 354-360. 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19960726)67:4<354::AID-AJMG7>3.0.CO;2-M.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  5. Kolevzon A, Smith CJ, Schmeidler J, Buxbaum JD, Silverman JM: Familial symptom domains in monozygotic siblings with autism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2004, 129B (1): 76-81. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Szatmari P, Merette C, Emond C, Zwaigenbaum L, Jones MB, Maziade M, Roy MA, Palmour R: Decomposing the autism phenotype into familial dimensions. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008, 147B (1): 3-9. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30561.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  7. Le Couteur A, Bailey A, Goode S, Pickles A, Robertson S, Gottesman I, Rutter M: A broader phenotype of autism: the clinical spectrum in twins. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1996, 37 (7): 785-801. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1996.tb01475.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  8. Hallmayer J, Cleveland S, Torres A, Phillips J, Cohen B, Torigoe T, Miller J, Fedele A, Collins J, Smith K, Lotspeich L, Croen LA, Ozonoff S, Lajonchere C, Grether JK, Risch N: Genetic heritability and shared environmental factors among twin pairs with autism. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011, 68 (11): 1095-1102. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.76.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  9. Sucksmith E, Roth I, Hoekstra RA: Autistic traits below the clinical threshold: re-examining the broader autism phenotype in the 21st century. Neuropsychol Rev. 2011, 21 (4): 360-389. 10.1007/s11065-011-9183-9.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  10. Szatmari P, MacLean JE, Jones MB, Bryson SE, Zwaigenbaum L, Bartolucci G, Mahoney WJ, Tuff L: The familial aggregation of the lesser variant in biological and nonbiological relatives of PDD probands: a family history study. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2000, 41 (5): 579-586. 10.1111/1469-7610.00644.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  11. Bolton P, Macdonald H, Pickles A, Rios P, Goode S, Crowson M, Bailey A, Rutter M: A case–control family history study of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1994, 35 (5): 877-900. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb02300.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Georgiades S, Szatmari P, Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Brian J, Roberts W, Smith I, Vaillancourt T, Roncadin C, Garon N: A prospective study of autistic-like traits in unaffected siblings of probands with autism spectrum disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013, 70 (1): 42-48. 10.1001/2013.jamapsychiatry.1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  13. Li X, Zou H, Brown WT: Genes associated with autism spectrum disorder. Brain Res Bull. 2012, 88 (6): 543-552. 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.05.017.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  14. Anney R, Klei L, Pinto D, Almeida J, Bacchelli E, Baird G, Bolshakova N, Bolte S, Bolton PF, Bourgeron T, Brennan S, Brian J, Casey J, Conroy J, Correia C, Corsello C, Crawford EL, De Jonge M, Delorme R, Duketis E, Duque F, Estes A, Farrar P, Fernandez BA, Folstein SE, Fombonne E, Gilbert J, Gillberg C, Glessner JT, Green A: Individual common variants exert weak effects on the risk for autism spectrum disorderspi. Hum Mol Genet. 2012, 21 (21): 4781-4792. 10.1093/hmg/dds301.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Ma D, Salyakina D, Jaworski JM, Konidari I, Whitehead PL, Andersen AN, Hoffman JD, Slifer SH, Hedges DJ, Cukier HN, Griswold AJ, McCauley JL, Beecham GW, Wright HH, Abramson RK, Martin ER, Hussman JP, Gilbert JR, Cuccaro ML, Haines JL, Pericak-Vance MA: A genome-wide association study of autism reveals a common novel risk locus at 5p14.1. Ann Hum Genet. 2009, 73 (Pt 3): 263-273.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  16. Wang K, Zhang H, Ma D, Bucan M, Glessner JT, Abrahams BS, Salyakina D, Imielinski M, Bradfield JP, Sleiman PM, Kim CE, Hou C, Frackelton E, Chiavacci R, Takahashi N, Sakurai T, Rappaport E, Lajonchere CM, Munson J, Estes A, Korvatska O, Piven J, Sonnenblick LI, Alvarez Retuerto AI, Herman EI, Dong H, Hutman T, Sigman M, Ozonoff S, Klin A: Common genetic variants on 5p14.1 associate with autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2009, 459 (7246): 528-533. 10.1038/nature07999.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  17. Weiss LA, Arking DE, Daly MJ, Chakravarti A, Gene Discovery Project of Johns Hopkins & the Autism Consortium: A genome-wide linkage and association scan reveals novel loci for autism. Nature. 2009, 461 (7265): 802-808. 10.1038/nature08490.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  18. Smoller JW, Craddock N, Kendler K, Lee PH, Neale BM, Nurnberger JI, Ripke S, Santangelo S, Sullivan PF, Cross-Disorder Group of the Psychiatric Genomics Consortium: Identification of risk loci with shared effects on five major psychiatric disorders: a genome-wide analysis. Lancet. 2013, 381 (9875): 1371-1379.

    Article  CAS  Google Scholar 

  19. Marshall CR, Noor A, Vincent JB, Lionel AC, Feuk L, Skaug J, Shago M, Moessner R, Pinto D, Ren Y, Thiruvahindrapduram B, Fiebig A, Schreiber S, Friedman J, Ketelaars CE, Vos YJ, Ficicioglu C, Kirkpatrick S, Nicolson R, Sloman L, Summers A, Gibbons CA, Teebi A, Chitayat D, Weksberg R, Thompson A, Vardy C, Crosbie V, Luscombe S, Baatjes R: Structural variation of chromosomes in autism spectrum disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2008, 82 (2): 477-488. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.12.009.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  20. Pinto D, Pagnamenta AT, Klei L, Anney R, Merico D, Regan R, Conroy J, Magalhaes TR, Correia C, Abrahams BS, Almeida J, Bacchelli E, Bader GD, Bailey AJ, Baird G, Battaglia A, Berney T, Bolshakova N, Bolte S, Bolton PF, Bourgeron T, Brennan S, Brian J, Bryson SE, Carson AR, Casallo G, Casey J, Chung BH, Cochrane L, Corsello C: Functional impact of global rare copy number variation in autism spectrum disorders. Nature. 2010, 466 (7304): 368-372. 10.1038/nature09146.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Zhao X, Leotta A, Kustanovich V, Lajonchere C, Geschwind DH, Law K, Law P, Qiu S, Lord C, Sebat J, Ye K, Wigler M: A unified genetic theory for sporadic and inherited autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2007, 104 (31): 12831-12836. 10.1073/pnas.0705803104.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  22. Matsunami N, Hadley D, Hensel CH, Christensen GB, Kim C, Frackelton E, Thomas K, Da Silva RP, Stevens J, Baird L, Otterud B, Ho K, Varvil T, Leppert T, Lambert CG, Leppert M, Hakonarson H: Identification of rare recurrent copy number variants in high-risk autism families and their prevalence in a large ASD population. PLoS One. 2013, 8 (1): e52239-10.1371/journal.pone.0052239.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  23. Fernandez BA, Roberts W, Chung B, Weksberg R, Meyn S, Szatmari P, Joseph-George AM, Mackay S, Whitten K, Noble B, Vardy C, Crosbie V, Luscombe S, Tucker E, Turner L, Marshall CR, Scherer SW: Phenotypic spectrum associated with de novo and inherited deletions and duplications at 16p11.2 in individuals ascertained for diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. J Med Genet. 2010, 47 (3): 195-203. 10.1136/jmg.2009.069369.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  24. Noor A, Whibley A, Marshall CR, Gianakopoulos PJ, Piton A, Carson AR, Orlic-Milacic M, Lionel AC, Sato D, Pinto D, Drmic I, Noakes C, Senman L, Zhang X, Mo R, Gauthier J, Crosbie J, Pagnamenta AT, Munson J, Estes AM, Fiebig A, Franke A, Schreiber S, Stewart AF, Roberts R, McPherson R, Guter SJ, Cook EH, Dawson G, Schellenberg GD: Disruption at the PTCHD1 Locus on Xp22.11 in Autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. Sci Transl Med. 2010, 2 (49): 49ra68-10.1126/scitranslmed.3001267.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Pagnamenta AT, Khan H, Walker S, Gerrelli D, Wing K, Bonaglia MC, Giorda R, Berney T, Mani E, Molteni M, Pinto D, Le Couteur A, Hallmayer J, Sutcliffe JS, Szatmari P, Paterson AD, Scherer SW, Vieland VJ, Monaco AP: Rare familial 16q21 microdeletions under a linkage peak implicate cadherin 8 (CDH8) in susceptibility to autism and learning disability. J Med Genet. 2011, 48 (1): 48-54. 10.1136/jmg.2010.079426.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Sato D, Lionel AC, Leblond CS, Prasad A, Pinto D, Walker S, O’Connor I, Russell C, Drmic IE, Hamdan FF, Michaud JL, Endris V, Roeth R, Delorme R, Huguet G, Leboyer M, Rastam M, Gillberg C, Lathrop M, Stavropoulos DJ, Anagnostou E, Weksberg R, Fombonne E, Zwaigenbaum L, Fernandez BA, Roberts W, Rappold GA, Marshall CR, Bourgeron T, Szatmari P, Scherer SW: SHANK1 Deletions in Males with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2012, 90 (5): 879-887. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2012.03.017.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  27. Vaags AK, Lionel AC, Sato D, Goodenberger M, Stein QP, Curran S, Ogilvie C, Ahn JW, Drmic I, Senman L, Chrysler C, Thompson A, Russell C, Prasad A, Walker S, Pinto D, Marshall CR, Stavropoulos DJ, Zwaigenbaum L, Fernandez BA, Fombonne E, Bolton PF, Collier DA, Hodge JC, Roberts W, Szatmari P, Scherer SW: Rare deletions at the neurexin 3 locus in autism spectrum disorder. Am J Hum Genet. 2012, 90 (1): 133-141. 10.1016/j.ajhg.2011.11.025.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  28. Allen-Brady K, Cannon D, Robison R, McMahon WM, Coon H: A unified theory of autism revisited: linkage evidence points to chromosome X using a high-risk subset of AGRE families. Autism Res. 2010, 3 (2): 47-52.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  29. Coon H, Matsunami N, Stevens J, Miller J, Pingree C, Camp NJ, Thomas A, Krasny L, Lainhart J, Leppert MF, McMahon W: Evidence for linkage on chromosome 3q25-27 in a large autism extended pedigree. Hum Hered. 2005, 60 (4): 220-226. 10.1159/000090546.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  30. Coon H, Villalobos ME, Robison RJ, Camp NJ, Cannon DS, Allen-Brady K, Miller JS, McMahon WM: Genome-wide linkage using the Social Responsiveness Scale in Utah autism pedigrees. Mol Autism. 2010, 1 (1): 8-10.1186/2040-2392-1-8.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  31. Kilpinen H, Ylisaukko-oja T, Rehnstrom K, Gaal E, Turunen JA, Kempas E, Von Wendt L, Varilo T, Peltonen L: Linkage and linkage disequilibrium scan for autism loci in an extended pedigree from Finland. Hum Mol Genet. 2009, 18 (15): 2912-2921. 10.1093/hmg/ddp229.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  32. Salyakina D, Cukier HN, Lee JM, Sacharow S, Nations LD, Ma D, Jaworski JM, Konidari I, Whitehead PL, Wright HH, Abramson RK, Williams SM, Menon R, Haines JL, Gilbert JR, Cuccaro ML, Pericak-Vance MA: Copy number variants in extended autism spectrum disorder families reveal candidates potentially involved in autism risk. PLoS One. 2011, 6 (10): e26049-10.1371/journal.pone.0026049.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  33. The Declaration of Helsinki. World Med J. 1982, 29 (6): 86-88.

  34. Piven J, Gayle J, Chase GA, Fink B, Landa R, Wzorek MM, Folstein SE: A family history study of neuropsychiatric disorders in the adult siblings of autistic individuals. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1990, 29 (2): 177-183. 10.1097/00004583-199003000-00004.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  35. Lord C, Rutter M, Le Couteur A: Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised: a revised version of a diagnostic interview for caregivers of individuals with possible pervasive developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 1994, 24 (5): 659-685. 10.1007/BF02172145.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  36. Gotham K, Risi S, Pickles A, Lord C: The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule: revised algorithms for improved diagnostic validity. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007, 37 (4): 613-627. 10.1007/s10803-006-0280-1.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  37. American Psychiatric Association: Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 1994, Washington DC: APA, 4

    Google Scholar 

  38. Risi S, Lord C, Gotham K, Corsello C, Chrysler C, Szatmari P, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, Pickles A: Combining information from multiple sources in the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2006, 45 (9): 1094-1103. 10.1097/01.chi.0000227880.42780.0e.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Losh M, Adolphs R, Poe MD, Couture S, Penn D, Baranek GT, Piven J: Neuropsychological profile of autism and the broad autism phenotype. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2009, 66 (5): 518-526. 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.34.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  40. Piven J, Palmer P, Landa R, Santangelo S, Jacobi D, Childress D: Personality and language characteristics in parents from multiple-incidence autism families. Am J Med Genet. 1997, 74 (4): 398-411. 10.1002/(SICI)1096-8628(19970725)74:4<398::AID-AJMG11>3.0.CO;2-D.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  41. Piven J, Wzorek M, Landa R, Lainhart J, Bolton P, Chase GA, Folstein S: Personality characteristics of the parents of autistic individuals. Psychol Med. 1994, 24 (3): 783-795. 10.1017/S0033291700027938.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  42. Tyrer P: Personality Disorders: Diagnosis, Management and Course. Edited by: Tyrer P. 1988, London: Butterworth, 140-167. Personality assessment schedule, Tyrer P

    Google Scholar 

  43. Landa R, Piven J, Wzorek MM, Gayle JO, Chase GA, Folstein SE: Social language use in parents of autistic individuals. Psychol Med. 1992, 22 (1): 245-254. 10.1017/S0033291700032918.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  44. Losh M, Childress D, Lam K, Piven J: Defining key features of the broad autism phenotype: a comparison across parents of multiple- and single-incidence autism families. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2008, 147B (4): 424-433. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30612.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  45. Hurley RS, Losh M, Parlier M, Reznick JS, Piven J: The broad autism phenotype questionnaire. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007, 37 (9): 1679-1690. 10.1007/s10803-006-0299-3.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  46. Sasson NJ, Lam KS, Childress D, Parlier M, Daniels JL, Piven J: The Broad Autism Phenotype Questionnaire: prevalence and diagnostic classification. Autism Res. 2013, 6 (2): 134-143. 10.1002/aur.1272.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Costa PJ, McCrae R: Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R). 1994, Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc.

    Google Scholar 

  48. Lam KS, Aman MG: The Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised: independent validation in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. 2007, 37 (5): 855-866. 10.1007/s10803-006-0213-z.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  49. Constantino J: The Social Responsiveness Scale. 2002, Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services

    Google Scholar 

  50. Semel E, Wiig E, Secord W: Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Fourth Edition. 2003, San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation

    Google Scholar 

  51. Wagner RK, Torgesen JK, Rashotte CA: Comprehensive Test of Phonological Processing. 1999, Austin, TX: PRO-ED

    Google Scholar 

  52. Piven J, Palmer P: Cognitive deficits in parents from multiple-incidence autism families. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1997, 38 (8): 1011-1021. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01618.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  53. Matise TC, Chen F, Chen W, De La Vega FM, Hansen M, He C, Hyland FC, Kennedy GC, Kong X, Murray SS, Ziegle JS, Stewart WC, Buyske S: A second-generation combined linkage physical map of the human genome. Genome Res. 2007, 17 (12): 1783-1786. 10.1101/gr.7156307.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  54. Vieland VJ, Huang Y, Seok SC, Burian J, Catalyurek U, O’Connell J, Segre A, Valentine-Cooper W: KELVIN: a software package for rigorous measurement of statistical evidence in human genetics. Hum Hered. 2011, 72 (4): 276-288. 10.1159/000330634.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  55. Huang Y, Segre A, O’Connell J, Wang H, Vieland VJ: KELVIN: A 2nd Generation Distributed Multiprocessor Linkage and Linkage Disequilibrium Analysis Program. In American Society of Human Genetics 56th Annual Meeting. 2006, Oxford: KELVIN

    Google Scholar 

  56. Huang Y, Thomas A, Vieland VJ: Employing MCMC under the PPL framework to analyze sequence data in large pedigrees. Front Genet. 2013, 4: 59-

    PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  57. Seok SC, Evans M, Vieland VJ: Fast and accurate calculation of a computationally intensive statistic for mapping disease genes. J Comput Biol. 2009, 16 (5): 659-676. 10.1089/cmb.2008.0175.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  58. Smith CA: Testing for heterogeneity of recombination fraction values in human genetics. Ann Hum Genet. 1963, 27: 175-182. 10.1111/j.1469-1809.1963.tb00210.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  59. Bartlett CW, Vieland VJ: Accumulating quantitative trait linkage evidence across multiple datasets using the posterior probability of linkage. Genet Epidemiol. 2007, 31 (2): 91-102. 10.1002/gepi.20193.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  60. Govil M, Vieland VJ: Practical considerations for dividing data into subsets prior to PPL analysis. Hum Hered. 2008, 66 (4): 223-237. 10.1159/000143405.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  61. Elston RC, Lange K: The prior probability of autosomal linkage. Ann Hum Genet. 1975, 38 (3): 341-350. 10.1111/j.1469-1809.1975.tb00619.x.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  62. Royall R: Statistical Evidence: A Likelihood Paradigm. 1997, London: Chapman and Hall

    Google Scholar 

  63. Vieland VJ: Review of statistical evidence: a likelihood paradigm. Am J Hum Genet. 1998, 63: 283-289.

    Google Scholar 

  64. Vieland VJ: Thermometers: something for statistical geneticists to think about. Hum Hered. 2006, 61 (3): 144-156. 10.1159/000093775.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  65. Weeks DE, Lehner T, Squires-Wheeler E, Kaufmann C, Ott J: Measuring the inflation of the lod score due to its maximization over model parameter values in human linkage analysis. Genet Epidemiol. 1990, 7 (4): 237-243. 10.1002/gepi.1370070402.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  66. Wassink TH, Piven J, Vieland VJ, Jenkins L, Frantz R, Bartlett CW, Goedken R, Childress D, Spence MA, Smith M, Sheffield VC: Evaluation of the chromosome 2q37.3 gene CENTG2 as an autism susceptibility gene. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2005, 136B (1): 36-44. 10.1002/ajmg.b.30180.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  67. Johansson M, Gillberg C, Rastam M: Autism spectrum conditions in individuals with Mobius sequence, CHARGE syndrome and oculo-auriculo-vertebral spectrum: diagnostic aspects. Res Dev Disabil. 2010, 31 (1): 9-24. 10.1016/j.ridd.2009.07.011.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  68. Liu XQ, Georgiades S, Duku E, Thompson A, Devlin B, Cook EH, Wijsman EM, Paterson AD, Szatmari P: Identification of genetic loci underlying the phenotypic constructs of autism spectrum disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2011, 50 (7): 687-696. 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.05.002. e13

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  69. Lam KS, Bodfish JW, Piven J: Evidence for three subtypes of repetitive behavior in autism that differ in familiality and association with other symptoms. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2008, 49 (11): 1193-1200. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2008.01944.x.

    Article  PubMed Central  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  70. Wolff JJ, Bodfish JW, Hazlett HC, Lightbody AA, Reiss AL, Piven J: Evidence of a distinct behavioral phenotype in young boys with fragile X syndrome and autism. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psych. 2012, 51 (12): 1324-1332. 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.09.001.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Jones MB, Szatmari P: A risk-factor model of epistatic interaction, focusing on autism. Am J Med Genet. 2002, 114 (5): 558-565. 10.1002/ajmg.10513.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Leblond CS, Heinrich J, Delorme R, Proepper C, Betancur C, Huguet G, Konyukh M, Chaste P, Ey E, Rastam M, Anckarsater H, Nygren G, Gillberg IC, Melke J, Toro R, Regnault B, Fauchereau F, Mercati O, Lemiere N, Skuse D, Poot M, Holt R, Monaco AP, Jarvela I, Kantojarvi K, Vanhala R, Curran S, Collier DA, Bolton P, Chiocchetti A: Genetic and functional analyses of SHANK2 mutations suggest a multiple hit model of autism spectrum disorders. PLoS Genet. 2012, 8 (2): e1002521-10.1371/journal.pgen.1002521.

    Article  PubMed Central  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Piven J: The broad autism phenotype: a complementary strategy for molecular genetic studies of autism. Am J Med Genet. 2001, 105 (1): 34-35. 10.1002/1096-8628(20010108)105:1<34::AID-AJMG1052>3.0.CO;2-D.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

This project was supported by the following grants: NIH MH076028, HD003110 (JP), R01 MH086117 (VJV); U24 MH068457 (J Tischfield, PI). We also acknowledge the contributions of Molly Losh, Ph.D. to the design of this study. Finally, we would like to thank all the families who participated in this research.

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Joseph Piven.

Additional information

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Authors’ contributions

JP, VJV and PS conceived of the study, participated in design and coordination, and performed data interpretation and manuscript preparation; BF, M W-S, IO, MP and AT participated in the design of the study, oversaw data collection, and aided manuscript preparation; YH was involved in development and application of computer programs used for the analyses; KW performed data analysis. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Joseph Piven, Veronica J Vieland and Peter Szatmari contributed equally to this work.

Authors’ original submitted files for images

Rights and permissions

This article is published under license to BioMed Central Ltd. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/2.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Piven, J., Vieland, V.J., Parlier, M. et al. A molecular genetic study of autism and related phenotypes in extended pedigrees. J Neurodevelop Disord 5, 30 (2013). https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-5-30

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/1866-1955-5-30

Keywords